Abstract
Background
Many of our efforts in social interactions are dedicated to learning about others.
Adolescents with autism have core deficits in social learning, but a mechanistic understanding
of these deficits and how they relate to neural development is lacking. The present
study aimed to specify how adolescents with and without autism represent and acquire
social knowledge and how these processes are implemented in neural activity.
Methods
Typically developing adolescents (n = 26) and adolescents with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) (n = 20) rated in the magnetic resonance scanner how much 3 peers liked a variety of
items and received trial-by-trial feedback about the peers’ actual preference ratings.
In a separate study, we established the preferences of a new sample of adolescents
(N = 99), used to examine population preference structures. Using computational models,
we tested whether participants in the magnetic resonance study relied on preference
structures during learning and how model predictions were implemented in brain activity.
Results
Typically developing adolescents relied on average population preferences and prediction
error updating. Importantly, prediction error updating was scaled by the similarity
between items. In contrast, preferences of adolescents with ASD were best described
by a No-Learning model that relied only on the participant’s own preferences for each
item. Model predictions were encoded in neural activity. Typically developing adolescents
encoded prediction errors in the putamen, and adolescents with ASD showed greater
encoding of own preferences in the angular gyrus.
Conclusions
We specified how adolescents represent and update social knowledge during learning.
Our findings indicate that adolescents with ASD rely only on their own preferences
when making social inferences.
Keywords
To read this article in full you will need to make a payment
Purchase one-time access:
Academic & Personal: 24 hour online accessCorporate R&D Professionals: 24 hour online accessOne-time access price info
- For academic or personal research use, select 'Academic and Personal'
- For corporate R&D use, select 'Corporate R&D Professionals'
Subscribe:
Subscribe to Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and NeuroimagingAlready a print subscriber? Claim online access
Already an online subscriber? Sign in
Register: Create an account
Institutional Access: Sign in to ScienceDirect
References
- Is adolescence a sensitive period for sociocultural processing?.Annu Rev Psychol. 2014; 65: 187-207
- A social neuroscience perspective on adolescent risk-taking.Dev Rev. 2008; 28: 78-106
- The social brain in adolescence: Evidence from functional magnetic resonance imaging and behavioural studies.Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2011; 35: 1654-1664
- The adolescent brain.Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2008; 1124: 111-126
- Development of rostral prefrontal cortex and cognitive and behavioural disorders.Dev Med Child Neurol. 2008; 50: 168-181
- Adolescent brain development in normality and psychopathology.Dev Psychopathol. 2013; 25: 1325-1345
- Brain development during childhood and adolescence: A longitudinal MRI study.Nat Neurosci. 1999; 2: 861-863
- Special issue on the teenage brain: Sensitivity to social evaluation.Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2013; 22: 121-127
- A computational account of optimizing social predictions reveals that adolescents are conservative learners in social contexts.J Neurosci. 2018; 38: 974-988
- The medial prefrontal cortex and the emergence of self-conscious emotion in adolescence.Psychol Sci. 2013; 24: 1554-1562
- Charting the typical and atypical development of the social brain.Dev Psychopathol. 2008; 20: 1081-1102
- The association between theory of mind, executive function, and the symptoms of autism spectrum disorder.Autism Res. 2018; 11: 95-109
- Getting answers from babies about autism.Trends Cogn Sci. 2010; 14: 81-87
- Children with autism fail to orient to naturally occurring social stimuli.J Autism Dev Disord. 1998; 28: 479-485
- The enactive mind, or from actions to cognition: Lessons from autism.Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci. 2003; 358: 345-360
- More thoughts on the unconscious: Reply to Brody and to Lewicki and Hill.J Exp Psychol Gen. 1989; 118: 242-244
- Efficiency and conceptual fluency as independent mechanisms in implicit learning.Diss Abstr Int Sect B Sci Eng. 1995; 55: 5107
- On the relationship between task performance and associated verbalizable knowledge.Q J Exp Psychol Sect A. 1984; 36: 209-231
- Social Cognition: From Brains to Culture.3rd ed. Sage Publications, 2016
- Motivational and emotional aspects of the self.Annu Rev Psychol. 2007; 58: 317-344
- Adolescence, stress and cortisol in autism spectrum disorders.OA Autism. 2013; 1: 2
- The symptoms of autism spectrum disorders in adolescence and adulthood.J Autism Dev Disord. 2003; 33: 565-581
- Cooperation and heterogeneity of the autistic mind.J Neurosci. 2010; 30: 8815-8818
- Social Bayes: Using Bayesian modeling to study autistic trait–related differences in social cognition.Biol Psychiatry. 2016; 80: 112-119
- Development of the adolescent brain: Implications for executive function and social cognition.J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2006; 47: 296-312
- Decision-making in the adolescent brain.Nat Neurosci. 2012; 15: 1184-1191
- AIC model selection using Akaike weights.Psychon Bull Rev. 2004; 11: 192-196
- Ten simple rules for the computational modeling of behavioral data.Elife. 2019; 8: e49547
- Adolescent-specific patterns of behavior and neural activity during social reinforcement learning.Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2014; 14: 683-697
- Social Responsiveness Scale (SRS).Western Psychological Services. 2005;
- A direct approach to false discovery rates.J R Stat Soc Ser B Stat Methodol. 2002; 64: 479-498
- An integrative neural model of social perception, action observation, and theory of mind.Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2015; 51: 263-275
- Approximating implicit and explicit mentalizing with two naturalistic video-based tasks in typical development and autism spectrum disorder.J Autism Dev Disord. 2015; 45: 953-965
- Online usage of theory of mind continues to develop in late adolescence.Dev Sci. 2010; 13: 331-338
- Cognitive, language, social and behavioural outcomes in adults with autism spectrum disorders: A systematic review of longitudinal follow-up studies in adulthood.Clin Psychol Rev. 2014; 34: 73-86
- Adult outcome for children with autism.J Child Psychol Psychiatry Allied Discip. 2004; 45: 212-229
- Maturation of social attribution skills in typically developing children: An investigation using the social attribution task.Behav Brain Funct. 2010; 6: 10
- Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”?.Cognition. 1985; 21: 37-46
- A map of abstract relational knowledge in the human hippocampal-entorhinal cortex.Elife. 2017; 6e17086
- Organizing conceptual knowledge in humans with a gridlike code.Science. 2016; 352: 1464-1468
- Autism as a disorder of prediction.Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2014; 111: 15220-15225
- When the world becomes “too real”: A Bayesian explanation of autistic perception.Trends Cogn Sci. 2012; 16: 504-510
- On hyperpriors and hypopriors: Comment on Pellicano and Burr.Trends Cogn Sci. 2013; 17: 1
- Precise minds in uncertain worlds: Predictive coding in autism.Psychol Rev. 2014; 121: 649-675
- A unique adolescent response to reward prediction errors.Nat Neurosci. 2010; 13: 669-671
- Prediction error in reinforcement learning: A meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies.Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2013; 37: 1297-1310
- States versus rewards: Dissociable neural prediction error signals underlying model-based and model-free reinforcement learning.Neuron. 2010; 66: 585-595
- Different neural correlates of reward expectation and reward expectation error in the putamen and caudate nucleus during stimulus-action-reward association learning.J Neurophysiol. 2006; 95: 948-959
- The caudal part of putamen represents the historical object value information.J Neurosci. 2019; 39: 1709-1719
- Value computations in ventral medial prefrontal cortex during charitable decision making incorporate input from regions involved in social cognition.J Neurosci. 2010; 30: 583-590
- Learning-induced plasticity in medial prefrontal cortex predicts preference malleability.Neuron. 2015; 85: 418-428
- Associative learning of social value.Nature. 2008; 456: 245-249
- The angular gyrus: Multiple functions and multiple subdivisions.Neuroscientist. 2013; 19: 43-61
- Social learning through prediction error in the brain.NPJ Sci Learn. 2017; 2: 8
- Gray matter abnormalities in pediatric autism spectrum disorder: A meta-analysis with signed differential mapping.Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2017; 26: 933-945
- Neocerebellar contributions to social perception in adolescents with autism spectrum disorder.Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2014; 10: 77-92
- Neurobiological mechanisms of social cognition treatment in high-functioning adults with autism spectrum disorder.Psychol Med. 2019; : 1-11
- The social brain in adolescence.Nat Rev Neurosci. 2008; 9: 267-277
- Representational geometry: Integrating cognition, computation, and the brain.Trends Cogn Sci. 2013; 17: 401-412
- Using second-person neuroscience to elucidate the mechanisms of social interaction.Nat Rev Neurosci. 2019; 20: 495-505
Article info
Publication history
Published online: July 14, 2020
Accepted:
June 6,
2020
Received in revised form:
June 1,
2020
Received:
February 14,
2020
Identification
Copyright
© 2020 Society of Biological Psychiatry. Published by Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
ScienceDirect
Access this article on ScienceDirectLinked Article
- Studying Social Inferences in and Across Social BrainsBiological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and NeuroimagingVol. 6Issue 8
- PreviewPsychiatric disorders are ubiquitously characterized by social interaction difficulties, which has led to the suggestion that they could be construed as “disorders of social interaction” (1). Social impairments, indeed, can be found transdiagnostically and may precede the onset of other symptoms, but can also be a primary characteristic, such as in the case of developmental disorders. Developmental trajectories vary significantly across individuals and are shaped by both internal and external factors that affect social learning and its outcomes.
- Full-Text
- Preview